Research Article - Neuropsychiatry (2018) Volume 8, Issue 1
Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor Val66Met Polymorphism in Contex of Executive Functions and Working Memory in Obese Patients
- *Corresponding Author:
- Maciej Bieliński
Chair and Department of Clinical Neuropsychology
Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń
Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz, Poland
Tel: +48 52 585 37 03
Fax: +48 52 585 37 03
Abstract
ABSTRACT
Neurothrophins are crucial in the regulation of many processes, such as nerve cellsdevelopment and neuroplasticity and brain-derived neurothrophic factor (BDNF) is the major one. Researchers still scrutinize the role of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism in eating disorders and cognition, however, literature present inconsistent findings regarding this topic. Most of all, the exact mechanism of how exactly BDNF influences cognitive functioning in obesity is unknown. In our study, we try to elucidate the association of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism with executive functions and working memory on large obese population.
375 obese patients underwent biometric analyses and neuropsychological assessment. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) was used to assess cognitive functions. Genetic polymorphism in BDNF was evaluated in peripheral blood samples.
Keywords
Val66Met BDNF, Neurothrophins, Obesity
Introduction
Neurotrophins play a major role in the physiology of central nervous system (CNS). Among them, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and its TrkB receptor are the most widespread in the maturing and adult brains of all mammals [1,2]. BDNF is distributed throughout the whole CNS, and it is abundant in such regions as hippocampus, hypothalamus, cerebral cortex and neuronal pathways e.g. frontal-striatal circuit [3-5]. BDNF takes part in many processes which involve proper brain physiology and functioning: it participates in neuronal survival (proliferation, differentiation, maturation and apoptosis) and initiates synaptic transmission [6,7].
The human BDNF gene is located on chromosome 11p14.1 [8]. The most recent investigations examining BDNF gene polymorphisms frequently feature a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) Val66Met. This polymorphism is located on 66 codon of proBDNF and involves the conversion of valine to methionine in the pro-region of the 5’ end of the gene. This impairs intracellular trafficking and packaging of pro-BDNF and is associated with defective BDNF depolarization-induced secretion and thus lower BDNF serum concentration [9-11].
Building on BDNF concentration in CNS, Val66Met polymorphism may contribute to the development of eating disorders and cognition impairment. In context of obesity, many studies show the influence of BDNF on food intake, feeding behavior, body mass index (BMI) and the pathogenesis of eating disorders [12- 18]. However, the exact mechanism by which BDNF affects appetite, and hence contribute to development of obesity is still unknown [19]. Moreover, it is questionable which BDNF allele (Val or Met) is responsible for greater weight gain.
Literature also presents mixed results regarding the link between BDNF gene polymorphism and executive functions and working memory. In some studies Met allele is considered to deteriorate cognitive functioning, while other studies do not present any significant associations between BDNF Val66Met and cognition [9,20- 22].
To elucidate abovementioned uncertainties, the aim of our research was: to assess executive function performance in obese population, determine what factor is mainly responsible for the worse scores in WCST, and how BDNF Val66Met influences the severity of obesity.
Materials and Methods
▪ Participants
A total of 375 adult Caucasian patients of Polish nationality were enrolled in this study, including 233 women and 142 men, after excluding secondary reasons of obesity. The mean age of all participants was 54 (range, 20–76) years old; the mean age for women was 52 (20-75) years old, and for men 56 (42-67) years old. The exclusion criteria were individuals with a severe somatic or psychiatric disorder included in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, any neurological abnormality, or any addiction to drugs or alcohol. All participants were informed about the aims of this study and gave their written informed consent. Permission for the study was obtained from the Bioethical Commission of the NCU, Collegium Medium in Bydgoszcz (No. 533/2008) and the study conforms to recognized standards of Declaration of Helsinki.
▪ Clinical assessments and measures
All participants underwent clinical evaluation, which comprised a physical examination, medical history, metric measurements of body weight and height, and a neuropsychological assessment. For biometric analyses, adiposity was evaluated using BMI, which was calculated as a ratio between weight (kg) and height squared (m2). Primary obesity was defined as a BMI>30. Secondary causes of obesity were excluded based on medical history, physical examination, and biochemical results, including cortisol, prolactin, and thyroid-stimulating hormone levels.
The computerized version of the WCST with instructions in Polish was used to evaluate the function of the frontal lobe. The following psychometric parameters were measured: (1) percentage of perseverative errors (WCST_P); (2) percentage of non-perseverative errors (WCST_NP); (3) number of correctly completed categories (WCST_CC); (4) number of trials required to finish the first category (WCST_1st); (5) percentage of correct responses occurring in runs of three or more (WCST_CLR).
▪ Genotyping
Genomic DNA was extracted from 7-10 mL of peripheral blood using the method of Lahiri and Schnabel (1993) [23]. Blood was collected and mixed with 0.5 mL of 0.5 ethylene diamine tetraacetic acid, frozen in liquid nitrogen, and stored at −80°C prior to extraction. BDNF genotypes were determined by polymerase chain reaction (PCR). The following primers were used: BDNF forward, 5′-TTCTCCCTACAGTTCCACCAG-3′; BDNF reverse, 5′-GTTTCCCTTCTGGTCATGGA-3′. The expanded fragment was 963 bp long and covered sector 27619182-27620144, where polymorphism Val/Met was in position 27619916. PCR products were subjected to restriction digestion (2 h/37°C) using the enzyme Eco72I (Fermentas). Products with the CACGTG sequence corresponding to the 66/V allele were digested with 228 and 735 bp fragments. Products with the CACATG sequence corresponding to the 66 /Met allele retained their length and were not subject to cutting.
▪ Statistical analysis
A Shapiro–Wilk test revealed that the distribution of variables was not normal; therefore, nonparametric tests were used in subsequent analyses. The statistical significance of the differences was calculated using Mann–Whitney U tests. The Spearman rank correlation test was used to determine correlations between variables. Kendall’s partial rank correlation coefficient method was used to determine significance in cases of complex variables. Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was performed to assess interaction effects. Statistical 12.5 was used to conduct statistical analyses, and the software program Utility Programs for Analysis of Genetic Linkage was utilized to test the goodness of fit to the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium. A χ2 value of 35 (p value<0.0001) indicated that the study population was not in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium.
Results
The study population consisted of 375 individuals with a diagnosis of simple obesity. The median age was 45.2 (SD, 9.5) years in the study group and 40.4 (SD, 12.9) years in the control group. There were 233 women and 142 men in the study group. The results of the WCST and basic demographic factors are presented in Table 1. Significant differences were found between women and men in BMI and two WCST parameters, namely, in the number of perseverative errors and the conceptual level responses. Men scored significantly higher on WCST_P and WCST_CLR. Women were characterized by having BMIs significantly higher than those of the men.
Female n = 233 |
Male n = 142 |
p | Cohen’s d | Partial tau Kendall | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | Genotype | |||||
Age | 52.0 (40.0–69.0) |
56.0 (39.0–67.0) |
0.81 | 0.02 | ||
BMI | 37.2 (32.4–43.9) |
35.3 (30.8–44.0) |
0.04 | 0.08 | -0.42 p = 5.8E-35 |
0.067 p = 0.025 |
WCST_P | 12.0 (7.0–19.0) |
9.0 (6.0–19.0) |
0.004 | 0.03 | -0.152 p = 0.00001 |
-0.0116 p = 0.0007 |
WCST_NP | 14.0 (9.0–23.0) |
16.0 (9.0–19.0) |
0.21 | 0.15 | ||
WCST_CLR | 64.0 (42.0–76.0) |
73.0 (52.0–76.0) |
0.02 | 0.23 | 0.1256 p = 0.0002 |
0.0895 p = 0.009 |
WCST_CC | 6.0 (2.0–6.0) |
6.0 (3.0–6.0) |
0.76 | 0.09 | ||
WCST_1st | 18.0 (11.0–35.0) |
12.0 (11.0–31.0) |
0.17 | 0.01 |
Notes: Values are expressed as median (25%-75%) or as number of patients (n).Significance of differences between sexes was determined by the Mann–Whitney U test. Kendall’s partial rank correlation coefficient method indicates the significance of the individual factors. Size effect was measured by Cohen’s d method. Bold values indicate statistical significance.
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; WCST_P, percentage of perseverative errors; WCST_NP, percentage of nonperseverative errors; WCST_CLR, percentage of conceptual responses; WCST_CC, level of category completed; WCST_1st, number of cards needed to complete first category.
Table 1: Demographic factors and results on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in study participants.
The distribution of the BDNF polymorphism is shown in Table 2. A notable finding was that the study population consisted of no Met/Met carriers.
Val/Met n = 196 |
Val/Val n = 179 |
P | Cohen’s d | Partial tau Kendall | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BMI | Genotype | |||||
Gender | 116♀/80♂ | 117♀/62♂ | 0.22 | - | ||
BMI | 32.4 (30.8–40.1) |
42.7 (35.5–52.1) |
9.69E-17 | 0.64 | ||
WCST_P | 10.0 (5.0–19.0) |
12.0 (9.0–19.0) |
0.0002 | 0.24 | -0.14; p = 00006 |
0.02; p = 0.39 |
WCST_NP | 16.0 (7.0–19.0) |
14.0 (10.0–23.0) |
0.08 | 0.12 | ||
WCST_CLR | 72.0 (52.0–85.0) |
67.0 (42.0–76.0) |
0.01 | 0.21 | 0.08; p = 0.02 |
-0.07; p = 0.048 |
WCST_CC | 6.0 (4.0–6.0) |
6.0 (2.0–6.0) |
0.0007 | 0.47 | 0.17; p = 3.7E-07 |
0.07; p = 0.03 |
WCST_1st | 12.0 (11.0–27.0) |
14.0 (11.0–69.0) |
0.002 | 0.55 | -0.16; p = 0.00002 |
-0.1; p = 0.002 |
Notes: Values are expressed as median (25%-75%) or as number of patients (n).Significance of differences between genotypes was determined by the Mann–Whitney U test. Kendall’s partial rank correlation coefficient method indicates the significance of the individual factors. Size effect was measured by Cohen’s d method. Bold values indicate statistical significance.
Abbreviations: BDNF, Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor; BMI, body mass index; WCST_P, percentage of perseverative errors; WCST_NP, percentage of nonperseverative errors; WCST_CLR, percentage of conceptual responses; WCST_CC, level of category completed; WCST_1st, number of cards needed to complete first category.
Table 2: Body mass index and results on the Wisconsin Card Sorting test in different BDNF Val66Met polymorphism group.
The BMI results and WCST findings according to BDNF polymorphism are presented in Table 2.
The Val/Val carriers had significantly higher BMIs and worse results for most of the WCST parameters examined.
Using Kendall’s partial rank correlation coefficient technique, we found that both genotype and gender showed significant correlations with BMI, WCST_P, and WCST_ NP (Table 1).
Kendall’s partial rank correlation tests also determined that both genotype and BMI, after their mutual correlation had been partialled out, showed significant correlations with WCST_ CLR, WCST_CC, and WCST_1st (Table 2). The correlations involving BMI were statistically stronger than genotype for the results of the cognitive task.
An analysis was also performed when the group was divided into those with mild (n=227) and morbid (n=148) obesity (Table 3). Patients with severe obesity were characterized by younger age and worse outcomes in terms of WCST parameters: number of perseverative and non-perseverative errors as well as conceptual level response.
Mild obesity N=227 |
Morbid obesity N=148 |
P | Cohen’s d | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | 143(63%)/ 84(37%) |
90(60,1%)/ 58(39,9%) |
0,67 | - |
BMI | 32,4 (30,8 – 35,5) |
49,6 (43,4 – 56,5) |
0,00E-01 | 8,6 |
Age | 62,0 (52,0 – 70,0) |
43,0 (38,5 – 68,0) |
2,04,68E-21 | 0,23 |
BDNF (n,%) Val/Val//Val/Met |
84(37%)/ 143(63%) |
95(64,2%)/ 53(35,8%) |
2,69E-07 | - |
WCST_P | 10,0 (6,0 – 19,0) |
12,0 (8,0-19,0) |
0,025 | 0,18 |
WCST_NP | 12,0 (7,0 – 19,0) |
16,0 (10,0-24,0) |
0,002 | 1,27 |
WCST_CLR | 73,0 (52,0 – 81,0) |
62,0 (42,0 – 75,0) |
0,0001 | 2,8 |
WCST_CC | 6,0 (2,0 – 6,0) |
6,0 (3,0 – 6,0 |
0,25 | 17,7 |
WCST_1st | 21,0 (11,0 – 37,0) |
12,0 (11,0 – 25,0) |
0,06 | 0,39 |
Notes: Values are expressed as median (25%-75%) or as number of patients (n).Significance of differences between genotypes was determined by the Mann–Whitney U test. Size effect was measured by Cohen’s d method. Bold values indicate statistical significance.
Abbreviations: BDNF, Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor; BMI, body mass index; WCST_P, percentage of perseverative errors; WCST_NP, percentage of nonperseverative errors; WCST_CLR, percentage of conceptual responses; WCST_CC, level of category completed; WCST_1st, number of cards needed to complete first category.
Table 3: Demographic factors and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test results in mild and morbid obesity groups.
The Spearman rank correlation test indicated that BMI was significantly correlated with worse WCST scores (Table 4) as follows: WCST_P, WCST_NP, WCST_CC and WCST_1st. whereas age was associated with worse performance in the WCST_P, WCST_NP and WCST_CLR.
WCST_P | WCST_NP | WCST_CLR | WCST_CC | WCST_1st | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | -0,116 (p=0,02) |
0,148 (p=0,004) |
-0,157 (p=0,002) |
0,014 (p=0,78) |
0,070 (p=0,17) |
BMI | -0,126 (p=0,01) |
0,128 (p=0,01) |
-0,02 (p=0,70) |
-0,179 (p=0,0004) |
0,275 (p<0,0001) |
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; WCST_P, percentage of perseverative errors; WCST_NP, percentage of nonperseverative errors; WCST_CLR, percentage of conceptual responses; WCST_CC, level of category completed; WCST_1st, number of cards needed to complete first category.
Table 4: Age and BMI R-Spearman correlations with WCST parameters.
Significant interaction effects for WCST parameters were found for gender, age, BMI and investigated BDNF polymorphism (Table 5).
WSCT_P | WCST_NP | WCST_CLR | WCST_CC | WCST_1st | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | SS | 22,6 | 36,3 | 44,7 | 5,6 | 24,7 |
F | 4,8 | 9,8 | 8,8 | 3,1 | 6,9 | |
p | 3,6E-12 | 00E-01 | 7,49E-11 | 0,002 | 3,33E-16 | |
Age | SS | 26656 | 42332 | 52950 | 5215 | 29679 |
F | 5,9 | 12,5 | 12,4 | 2,8 | 8,8 | |
p | 1,1E-15 | 0,00E-01 | 0,00E-01 | 0,004 | 0,00E-01 | |
BMI | SS | 14109 | 18542 | 17374 | 2311 | 10255 |
F | 9,6 | 16,1 | 8,6 | 3,2 | 7,4 | |
p | 0,00E-01 | 0,00E-01 | 0,00E-01 | 0,001 | 0,00E-01 | |
BDNF | SS | 38 | 42 | 47 | 11,9 | 29 |
F | 10 | 11 | 9 | 7 | 8 | |
p | 0,00E-01 | 0,00E-01 | 0,00E-01 | 3,42E-08 | 0,00E-01 |
Notes: One-dimensional ANOVA F-test based on SS. Bold values indicate statistical significance.
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; WCST_P, percentage of perseverative errors; WCST_NP, percentage of nonperseverative errors; WCST_CLR, percentage of conceptual responses; WCST_CC, level of category completed; WCST_1st, number of cards needed to complete first category; ANOVA, analysis of variance; SS, sum of squares.
Table 5: Analyses of unidimensional interaction effects for WCST parameters.
Discussion
Neurodevelopmental theories are currently popular among researchers exploring the etiopathogenesis of mental illnesses. This interesting research stream is ideally suited to issues related to BDNF, a leading neurotrophin responsible for growth, maturation, and differentiation of neurons. The role of BDNF in eating disorders appears to be complex. On the one hand, BDNF in the brain acts as a satiety factor19; on the other hand, BDNF gene expression is dependent on leptin and cholecystokinin, both of which act on satiety and hunger.
However, some publications question the association between BDNF and obesity [24,25]. The study of 339 adult healthy Caucasians did not present any co-relations between BDNF Val66Met and possible weight gain. Results demonstrated that smoking was a significant factor which affected BMI [24].
Other studies show significant correlations of the Val66Met BDNF polymorphism in participants diagnosed with simple obesity, but exact effects of BDNF concentration on appetite and weight currently remain uncertain.
According to Beckers et al., carriers of the methionine allele have higher BMI, as well as increased risk of developing obesity [17]. The methionine allele is also associated with a greater risk of developing eating disorders, such as bulimia nervosa and binge-eating [26]. Also decreased BDNF levels have been reported in women with anorexia nervosa [14]. Some studies prove the connection between BDNF depletion and greater BMI, by demonstrating lower food intake and greater energy expenditure after administrating exogenous BDNF into the brain [27-29]. Kernie et al. studied mice having one functional copy of the BDNF gene and found greater weight gain in these mice than in the wild-type population. Significantly higher body weights were observed in 50% of the males and 27% of the females [4].
Some studies show that greater BDNF concentration may be associated with obesity. In the study on Monteleone et al. increased BDNF levels have been observed in the serum of obese women compared with those in healthy women. The level of BDNF has been positively correlated with BMI and weight [14]. Many studies show the correlation between Val homozygotes and greater BMI in comparison to Met carriers in both adults and children in many ethnicities [13,18,30,31]. Those results are consistent with our research.
The results on the WCST for participants who were homozygous for Val were significantly worse than those for participants who were heterozygous for Val/Met. The poorer results for the individuals homozygous for Val were unexpected because better cognitive performance has been widely associated in previous studies with the Val allele, for example, in children [32] the elderly [33] and patients with Parkinson disease [34].
The main mechanism for the effect of BDNF on cognitive performance is the promotion of enhanced synaptic potentials. Long-term potentiation, as a form of synaptic plasticity, is associated with a cell model of long-term memory. Extended potentiation occurs during the monosynaptic conduction of high frequency stimulation and increases the efficiency of synaptic transmission [35-37].
It is generally thought that the Met allele is associated with poor performance on cognitive tests. Worse outcome in episodic memory may be due to abnormal neuronal activation of the hippocampus caused by lower BDNF secretion in Met allele carriers [9]; in a study on obese population Met carriers performed worse than non-carriers in WCST and made more perseverative errors. Furthermore, MRI studies displayed differences in cortex thickness between obese and non-obese Met carriers, showing that the thinnest frontal cortex areas pertained to obese subjects. This strongly suggests the connection between obesity, BDNF Val66Met and executive functions [21].
However, in some researches higher BDNF concentration correlates with worse cognitive performance. Study examining 64 people with bipolar disorder, displayed significantly more non-perseverative errors in the group homozygous for Val allele [20]. Cunha et al. showed that moderate BDNF overexpression in rats may lead to impaired learning in spatial and instrumental memory tasks. Authors suggest that the excess of mature BDNF may lead to inhibition of synaptic circuitry involved in learning [38].
In the present study, we conducted further analyses using Kendall’s partial rank correlation coefficient method and determined that it was likely that higher body weight was associated with the poorer cognitive performance in the group homozygous for Val allele. Compared with the Val/Val group, the group heterozygous for Val/ Met had a lower median BMI and demonstrated better results on the WCST test parameters, particularly in terms of response rates consistent with logical concepts, indicating better use of new information and experience, as well as better control of thinking during the test. Excess body weight has long been considered a modifiable risk factor for cognitive dysfunction [39]. For example, more than 30 years ago, significant and extensive dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex was observed in extremely obese populations [40].
Table 3 showed that patients with morbid obesity despite younger age demonstrated worse scores in WCST_P, WCST_NP and WCST_CRL in comparison to the mild obese group. Obese population demonstrated altered executive functions with underlying dysfunction of frontal-subcortical circuit [41]. It may result in impaired decision making, response inhibition and cognitive flexibility [42,43]. Moreover, obesity affects the structure of the frontal lobe; obesity correlates with lowered thickness of cortical and subcortical areas. Abovementioned may be the explanation of worse executive functioning in such patients despite younger age, compared to mild obese group. The study of Marqués- Iturria et al. presented the co-relation between lower cortical thickness and obesity, as well as performing more perseverative errors in WCST test. Our study corroborates with those findings, as our subjects - with greater BMI - made more perseverative errors as well [21].
Negative correlation between BMI and executive functions increases with age. In our study, younger group, but more severely obese, showed worse executive functioning. The possible explanation might be, that greater weight affected the performance more than advanced age; especially that age-range does not differ significantly between those two groups [41].
In conclusion, our results give new insight into the connection between BDNF concentration, obesity and cognitive performance on large sample. The Val66Met BDNF polymorphism in the indirect manner effected the poorer cognitive functioning observed in obese individuals who were homozygous for Val, rather than heterozygous for Val/Met. The Val allele was associated with higher BMI, suggesting that excessive weight may have impaired the activity of the prefrontal cortex. We propose that by affecting weight BDNF Val66Met may contribute to worse cognitive functioning in obese participants. Our findings attach importance to the role of BDNF in cognition and eating disorders especially that the latter has become a serious health problem worldwide. Therefore better understanding of processes involved in eating disorders is crucial to develop new therapeutic options for patients.
Limitations
The main limitation of our study is the lack of a population homozygous for Met allele, which made it difficult to draw further specific conclusions. Second, our study does not include control non-obese group, so it may make it difficult to interpret the relationship between variables properly.
Acknowledgement
This research was supported by a grant from the Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education (Grant No. NN 402053136).
References
- Barde YA, Edgar D, Thoenen H. Purification of a new neurotrophic factor from mammalian brain. EMBO. J 1(5), 549-553 (1982).
- Murer MG, Yan Q, Raisman-Vozari R. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the control human brain, and in Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. Prog. Neurobiol 63(1), 71-124 (2001).
- Maisonpierre PC, Belluscio L, Friedman B, et al. NT-3, BDNF, and NGF in the developing rat nervoussystem: parallel as well as reciprocal patterns of expression. Neuron 5(4), 501-509 (1990).
- Kernie SG, Liebl DJ, Parada LF. BDNF regulates behaviour and locomotor activity in mice. EMBO. J 19(6), 1290-1300 (2000).
- Baroni A, Castellanos FX. Neuroanatomic and cognitive abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the era of 'high definition' neuroimaging. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 30, 1-8 (2015).
- Lu B, Nagappan G, Lu Y. BDNF and synaptic plasticity, cognitive function, and dysfunction. Handb. Exp. Pharmacol 220, 223-50 (2014).
- Bramham CR, Panja D. BDNF regulation of synaptic structure, function, and plasticity. Neuropharmacology 76 Pt C, 601-602 (2014).
- Hanson IM, Seawright A, van Heyningen V. The human BDNF gene maps between FSHB and HVBS1 at the boundary of 11p13-p14. Genomics 13(4), 1331-1333 (1992).
- Egan MF, Kojima M, Callicott JH, et al. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell 112(2), 257-269 (2003).
- Su H, Tao J, Zhang J, et al. The Effects of BDNF Val66Met Gene Polymorphism on Serum BDNF and Cognitive Function in Methamphetamine-Dependent Patients and Normal Controls: A Case-Control Study. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol 35(5), 517-524 (2015).
- Chen ZY, Patel PD, Sant G, et al. Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Met66) alters the intracellular trafficking and activity-dependent secretion of wild-type BDNF in neurosecretory cells and cortical neurons. J. Neurosci, 24(18), 4401-4411 (2004).
- Rosas-Vargas H, Martínez-Ezquerro JD, Bienvenu T. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor, food intake regulation, and obesity. Arch. Med. Res 42(6), 482-494 (2011).
- Gunstad J, Schofield P, Paul RH, et al. BDNF Val66Met polymorphism is associated with body mass index in healthy adults. Neuropsychobiology 53(3), 153-156 (2006).
- Monteleone P, Tortorella A, Martiadis V, et al. Opposite changes in the serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor in anorexia nervosa and obesity. Psychosom. Med 66(5), 744-748 (2004).
- Ribasés M, Gratacòs M, Armengol L, et al. Met66 in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) precursor is associated with anorexia nervosa restrictive type. Mol. Psychiatry 8(8), 745-751 (2003).
- Skledar M, Nikolac M, Dodig-Curkovic K, et al. Association between brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met and obesity in children and adolescents. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 36(1), 136-140 (2012).
- Beckers S, Peeters A, Zegers D, et al. Association of the BDNF Val66Met variation with obesity in women. Mol. Genet. Metab 95(1-2), 110-112 (2008).
- Ma XY, Qiu WQ, Smith CE, et al. Association between BDNF rs6265 and obesity in the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study. J. Obes 102942 (2012).
- Unger TJ, Calderon GA, Bradley LC, et al. Selective deletion of Bdnf in the ventromedial and dorsomedial hypothalamus of adult mice results in hyperphagic behavior and obesity. J. Neurosci, 27(52), 14265-14274 (2007).
- Tramontina JF, Yates D, Magalhães PV et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene val66met polymorphism and executive functioning in patients with bipolar disorder. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr 31(2), 136-140 (2009).
- Marqués-Iturria I, Garolera M, Pueyo R, et al. The interaction effect between BDNF val66met polymorphism and obesity on executive functions and frontal structure. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet 165B(3), 245-253 (2014).
- Mandelman SD, Grigorenko EL. BDNF Val66Met and cognition: all, none, or some? A meta-analysis of the genetic association. Genes. Brain. Behav 11(2), 127-136 (2012).
- Lahiri DK, Schnabel B. DNA isolation by a rapid method from human blood samples: effects of MgCl2, EDTA, storage time, and temperature on DNA yield and quality. Biochem. Genet 31(7-8), 321-328 (1993).
- Nikolac Perkovic M, Mustapic M, Pavlovic M, et al. Lack of association between brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism and body mass index change over time in healthy adults. Neurosci. Lett 545,127-131 (2013).
- Zhao XY, Zhang MX, Cheng H, et al. [Risk of obesity-related gene polymorphism on the incidence and durative of childhood obesity]. Zhonghua. Liu. Xing. Bing. Xue. Za. Zhi, 34(6), 560-565 (2013).
- Gratacòs M, González JR, Mercader JM, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met and psychiatric disorders: meta-analysis of case-control studies confirm association to substance-relateddisorders, eating disorders, and schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 61(7), 911-922 (2007).
- Lapchak PA, Hefti F. BDNF and NGF treatment in lesioned rats: effects oncholinergic function and weight gain. Neuroreport 3(5), 405-408 (1992).
- Wang C, Bomberg E, Billington CJ, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the hypothalamic ventromedial nucleus increases energy expenditure. Brain. Res 1336, 66-77 (2010).
- Wang C, Bomberg E, Billington C, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus reduces energy intake. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol 293(3), R1003-12 (2007).
- Xi B, Wang C, Wu L, et al. Influence of physical inactivity on associations between single nucleotide polymorphisms and genetic predisposition to childhood obesity. Am. J. Epidemiol 173(11), 1256-1262 (2011).
- Timpano K.R., Schmidt N.B., Wheaton M.G., et al. Consideration of the BDNF gene in relation to two phenotypes: hoarding and obesity. J. Abnorm. Psychol 120(3), 700-707 (2011).
- Jasińska KK, Molfese PJ, Kornilov SA, et al. The BDNF Val66Met Polymorphism Influences Reading Ability and Patterns of Neural Activation in Children. PLoS. One 11(8), e0157449 (2016).
- Canivet A, Albinet CT, André N, et al. Effects of BDNF polymorphism and physical activity on episodic memory in the elderly: a cross sectional study. Eur. Rev. Aging. Phys. Act 12, 15 (2015).
- Altmann V, Schumacher-Schuh AF, Rieck M, et al. Val66Met BDNF polymorphism is associated with Parkinson's disease cognitive impairment. Neurosci. Lett 615, 88-91 (2016).
- Bliss TV & Collingridge GL. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361(6407), 31-39 (1993).
- Bliss T.V. & Gardner-Medwin A.R. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the unanaestetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J. Physiol 232(2), 357-374 (1973).
- Xie H, Leung KL, Chen L, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor rescues and prevents chronic intermittent hypoxia-induced impairment of hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity. Neurobiol. Dis 40(1), 155-162 (2010).
- Cunha C, Angelucci A, D'Antoni A, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) overexpression in the forebrain results in learning and memory impairments. Neurobiol. Dis 33(3), 358-368 (2009).
- Han C, Jo SA, Seo JA, et al. Adiposity parameters and cognitive function in the elderly: application of "Jolly Fat" hypothesis to cognition. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr 49(2), e133-8 (2009).
- Chelune G., Ortega D., Linton J. et al. Personality and cognitive findings among patients electing gastroplasty for morbid obesity. Int. J. Eat. Disord 5, 701–712 (1986).
- Stanek KM, Strain G, Devlin M et al. Body mass index and neurocognitive functioning across the adultlifespan. Neuropsychology 27(2), 141-151 (2013).
- Siervo M, Arnold R, Wells JC, et al. Intentional weight loss in overweight and obese individuals and cognitive function: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Rev, 12(11), 968–983 (2011).
- Smith E, Hay P, Campbell L, et al. A review of the association between obesity and cognitive function across the lifespan: implications for novel approaches to prevention and treatment. Obes. Rev 12(9), 740-755 (2011).