Research Article - Neuropsychiatry (2019) Volume 9, Issue 1

Evolution of BDNF Full-Length/Truncated Receptor Ratio and Cognitive/General Functioning After a First Episode of Psychosis

Corresponding Author:
Mónica Martínez-Cengotitabengoa
Department of Psychiatry, Araba University Hospital, Olaguibel Street 29, 01004, Vitoria, Spain
Tel: + 34 945007764
Fax: + 34 945007764

Abstract

Brain plasticity has demonstrated to play a role in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Cognitive deterioration in these patients can be prevented by ensuring the adequate functioning of signaling pathways associated with brain plasticity. As BDNF exerts its action through receptors, in this study, we hypothesized that levels of some BDNF receptors during a first episode of psychosis (FEP) would correlate with the cognitive and global functioning of patients in the long term. We also hypothesized that the improvement of the ratio of full-length (TrKB-FL) and truncated (TrKB-T) TrKB receptors, and the predominance of the full-length isoform would be associated with better cognition and functioning. Peripheral levels of full-length (TrKB-FL) and truncated (TrKB-T) TrKB receptors were assessed in a sample of 97 FEP patients and 97 matched healthy controls. TrKB-FL/TrKB-T ratio(hereinafter, FL/T) was calculated for each patient. Cognitive and global functioning was measured at inclusion and at two years. A high baseline FL/T ratio was found to be related to a better cognitive function (global cognition, verbal memory, working memory and premorbid IQ). Cognitive performance at disease onset and at two years improved when the levels of the ratio were higher than one, with functional BDNF receptor (TrKB-FL) exceeding the value of the truncated isoform (TrKB-T). In addition the increase in the FL/T ratio during the two years of follow-up had positive effects on global functioning. This may be due either to a reduction in TrKB-T or to an increase in TrKB-FL, or both. In conclusion FL / T ratio was related to general functioning and cognition in the long-term.

Keywords

Pathophysiology, Schizophrenia, Cognitive deterioration

Introduction

Schizophrenia has multiple etiologies. In most cases, its physiopathology is related to neurodevelopmental disorders [1]. Patients generally show some degree of cognitive impairment before and after the onset of the disease. One of the main hypotheses about the etiology of schizophrenia considers that it is related to alterations in brain plasticity [2]. Brain plasticity is provided by neurotrophic agents, being the Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) the most widely studied agent in patients with psychosis [24].

The cognitive function of patients with psychosis is impaired due to neurotrophic repair system dysfunction [5]. A recent study revealed that patients’ functioning and response to antipsychotic therapy are directly related to appropriate BDNF signaling [6]. In addition some meta-analyses demonstrate the role of BDNF in schizophrenia [3,7]. Conversely, its role in cognition is more controversial. While some studies report an association of BDNF with planning ability [8], reasoning and problem solving [9], no association was found in other studies [10]. BDNF binds, among others, to two types of cell surface receptors: a totally functional form (TrKB-FL) and a truncated nonfunctional form (TrkB-T) that impairs the repair function of BDNF by competing for binding to TrKBFL [11]. These receptors have been shown to play a crucial role in cognitive performance in rat models of schizophrenia [12] and Alzheimer’s disease[13]. Specifically, when BDNF/TrkB agonist was given to rats, their cognitive impairment improved. Nevertheless, to our knowledge, no studies have been performed to assess the relationship between BDNF receptors and cognitive function in patients with psychosis. Thus, we hypothesized that the levels of some BDNF receptors during a first episode of psychosis (FEP) would correlate with the cognitive and global functioning of patients in the long term. We also hypothesized that the improvement of the ratio of full-length (TrKBFL) and truncated (TrKB-T) TrKB receptors and predominance of the full-length isoform would be associated with better cognition and functioning.

The objective of this study was to determine whether the expression and evolution of some BDNF receptors are associated with the cognitive and global functioning of FEP patients in the long term (2 years). We also hypothesized that the improvement of the ratio of fulllength (TrKB-FL) and truncated (TrKB-T) TrKB receptors and predominance of the fulllength isoform would be associated with better cognition and functioning

Material and Methods

This study was performed within the framework of the longitudinal multicenter Flamm-PEPs study, which protocol has been described elsewhere [14]. The Flamm-PEPs study included 117 patients (aged between 9-35 years) with a FEP, and 106 age- and sex-matched healthy controls. The inclusion criteria for the group of patients were: 1) positive psychotic symptoms for less than 12 months, 2) Age of 9-35 at disease onset, 3) speaking Spanish fluently, and 4) giving written informed consent. Psychotic symptoms were assessed by the Positive and Negative Symptoms Scale (PANSS)[15]. Exclusion criteria for patients were: 1) mental retardation according to DSM-IV criteria, 2) history of head trauma with loss of consciousness, and 3) systemic disease with mental repercussions. Inclusion criteria for healthy controls were: 1) no past or current psychiatric disorder (DSMIV criteria), 2) speaking fluent Spanish, and 3) giving written informed consent. Exclusion criteria for healthy controls were the same as for patients plus a history of psychotic episodes in first-degree relatives. No participants had fever or any allergies, ongoing infections, or other serious physical conditions. No participants had received immunosuppressive drugs or vaccines in the last six months or anti-inflammatory drugs at least two days before blood sampling. In this sub study, we only included the 97 patients for whom baseline blood samples were available and who underwent neuropsychological assessment at baseline and at two years of follow-up. We also included 89 healthy controls, 55 of whom completed follow-up. As the sample of interest was the group of patients, the evolution of controls was not analyzed and their data were not included in longitudinal analyses. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committees of the six participating hospitals. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.

▪ Data Collection

After overnight fasting, venous blood was collected between 8 a.m. and 10 a.m. Blood collection and processing methods used in the Flamm-PEPs study are detailed at www.cibersam.es and have been described elsewhere[14]. Briefly, blood tubes were centrifuged (641g x 10 min, 4°C) for plasma extraction. Plasma was stored at -80°C. The remaining sample was diluted 1:2 in culture medium (Roswell Park Memorial Institute RPMI 1640, Invitrogen). A gradient with Ficoll-Paque was used to isolate mononuclear cells by centrifugation (800 g × 40 min, room temperature). PBMC layer was aspired and resuspended in RPMI and centrifuged (1116 g × 10 min, room temperature). The supernatant was removed and the mononuclear cell-enriched pellet was manually resuspended in RPMI and stored at ‑80°C.

BDNF expression levels in plasma were determined by enzymatic testing (Ray Biotech). Plasma BDNF levels reflect BDNF expression in the brain both in animal models and humans [16,17]. Full-length (TrKB-FL) and truncated (TrKB-T) receptor expression was determined in peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) by Western-Blot analysis. The relative expression of the two receptors was calculated as the TrKBFL/ TrkB-T ratio (hereinafter, FL/T ratio). Data of the FL/T ratio in this sample have been published previously [6].

Cognitive function was assessed in patients at baseline and at two years after disease onset by evaluating the following domains: speed of processing, attention, verbal memory, working memory, executive function and estimated premorbid intelligence coefficient (Figure 1). Cognitive Global Index (CGI) was calculated by averaging the scores on cognitive domains, except for estimated premorbid IQ, which was used as an adjustment variable in statistical analyses. Tests were administered by experienced psychologists by order of difficulty from least to most difficult to reduce the effect of fatigue and facilitate participant’s cooperation. Inter-rater reliability between two of the tests proved to be good to excellent (ICC >0.80). The two tests included the WAIS vocabulary subtest and the WCST, in which the final score may partially depend on the judgement of the rater. Finally, global functioning was evaluated using the GAF scale [18].

neuropsychiatry-cognitive-domain

Figure 1: Tests included in each cognitive domain.

▪ Statistical analysis

Multiple linear regressions were used to analyze the relationship between BDNF receptor expression, cognitive performance and global functioning. Regressions were adjusted for potential confounding variables such as gender, age, civil status, education, socioeconomic level, body mass index, antipsychotic treatment, diagnosis, tobacco, alcohol or other drugs. The final model was selected by a backwards stepwise procedure. Only the variables that caused a significant change in the regression coefficient for the independent variable or those found to be significant were maintained in the model. Finally, interactions were determined. Once the final model had been constructed, adjustment and normality of residuals were tested to determine the reliability of the model. In the case of the FL/T ratio, we used the quantitative variable for preliminary analysis and its qualitative form in order to explore the influence of the proportion of each receptor, taking 1 as threshold value. Regression data are expressed as β-coefficients and p-values. In the presence of significant interactions, stratified effect size (95% CI) on the dependent variable was calculated for different values of the independent variable (3rd, 25th, 50th, 75th and 97th percentiles).

The association between shifts in BDNF receptor expression and cognitive and functional performance at two years was assessed by repeated measures ANOVA.

Changes in cognitive domains from baseline to 2 years were evaluated by Student’s t-test for repeated measures.

Statistical analysis was performed using the Stata 12.1 software package. A p<0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Table 1 summarizes scores on the different cognitive domains evaluated both at baseline and at two years of follow-up. Data are presented as means and standard deviations. All values are Z scores except for premorbid IQs, which are presented as scalar scores.

  Patients Controls
  Baseline Two-year follow-up Statistic Baseline Two-year follow-up Statistic
Premorbid IQ  95.54 (14.63)  98.39 (14.43) t=-2.029, p= 0.047 108.14 (11.23) 111.00 (11.11) t=-1.123, p=0.266
Processing speed -0.74 (0.93) -0.45 (0.87) t=-3.129, p= 0.003 0.31 (0.76) 0.39 (0.66) t=-0.432, p=0.667
Attention  0.50 (0.73)  0.16 (0.84) t=3.423, p= 0.001 -0.09 (0.83) -0.17 (0.75) t=-0.265, p= 0.792
Verbal memory -0.95 (1.14) -0.43 (0.99) t=-3.994, p<0.001 0.05 (0.72) 0.42 (0.54) t=-4.342, p<0.001
Working memory -0.63 (0.72) -0.54 (0.87) t=-1.144, p= 0.257 0.08 (0.87) 0.20 (0.91) t=-0.817, p= 0.417
Executive function -0.43 (0.51) -0.24 (0.76) t=-1.055, p= 0.314 0.03 (0.28) 0.13 (0.34) t= -0.483, p= 0.637
Cognitive global index -2.21 (2.61) -1.38 (2.65) t=-0.818, p= 0.431 0.19 (0.37) 0.25 (0.26) t= -1.285, p= 0.221

Table 1: Scores on cognitive domains at disease onset and at 2 years.

In this sample of patients, plasma BDNF levels were not significantly related to cognitive performance. A high baseline FL/T ratio correlated with a better baseline cognitive function, as assessed by the cognitive global index (β=20.916, p=0.015) in FEP patients. No associations were observed in healthy controls (β=3.58, p=0.278).

The patients who experienced an increase in the FL/T ratio six months after FEP showed a better global functioning in the long term (as assessed by the GAF scale), as compared to patients who experienced a decrease in the FL/T ratio (F= 213.90, p<0.001) (Figure 2).

neuropsychiatry-BDNF-receptor

Figure 2: Evolution of functioning with respect to shifts in the BDNF receptor FL/T ratio.

As to specific cognitive domains, verbal memory was also related to a higher FL/T ratio (β=0.885, p=0.018). Interaction between FL/T ratio and educational level was significantly related to baseline IQ and working memory (β= -36.047, p= 0.003 and β= -1.523, p= 0.036 respectively).

Thus, in patients with a low to medium FL/T ratio, a higher educational level was significantly associated with a better baseline working memory and IQ (Table 2).

  Baseline IQ Baseline working memory
Percentiles (FL/T ratio value) Medium level
Effect size (CI)
High Level
Effect size (CI)
Medium level
Effect size (CI)
High Level
Effect size (CI)
P3 (0.18) 27.87 (10.75, 45) 41.38 (22.73, 60.03) 0.70 (-0.254, 1.654) 1.84 (0.814, 2.866)
P25 (0.67) 13.27 (4.32, 22.22) 23.72 (13.73, 33.71) 0.38 (-0.156, 0.916) 1.20 (0.606, 1.794)
P50 (0.90) 6.41 (-1.19, 14.02) 15.43 (6.65, 24.21) 0.20 (-0.261, 0.661) 0.85 (0.316, 1.384)
P75 (1.11) 0.15 (-8.84, 9.14) 7.86 (-2.56, 18.28) 0.04 (-0.516, 0.596) 0.53 (-0.117, 1.177)
P97 (1.96) -25.18 (-49.62, -0.74) -22.78 (-50.08, 4.52) -0.60 (-2.106, 0.906) -0.75 (-2.426, 0.926)

Table 2: Effect size of baseline FL/T on baseline cognitive function by educational level.

In light of the interaction observed between baseline FL/T and educational level, we investigated the impact of educational level on the cognitive function of FEP patients as a post-hoc result. After adjusting for IQ, we observed that completion of secondary or higher education studies was significantly associated with a faster speed of processing (β=0.483, p=0.030 and β=0.592, p=0.034). Furthermore, higher education was correlated with a better working memory (β=0.619, p=0.008).

As it occurred with baseline cognition, a higher FL/T ratio significantly correlated with a better overall cognitive function (β=70.769, p=0.003) and verbal memory (β=0.830, p=0.011) two years after the FEP.

No significant correlation was found between shifts in the FL/T ratio (as continuous variable) and cognitive performance. Nevertheless, when TrKB-FL/ TrKB-T ratio was higher than> 1 (as a qualitative variable when BDNF fulllength receptor levels exceed those of truncated receptors), cognition was better both at baseline (IQ: β=10.266, p=0.002; Processing speed: β=0.502, p= 0.029; CGI: β=18.339, p=0.006) and at two years (Verbal memory: β=0.620, p=0.020; CGI: β=28.488, p=0.004). This effect can be due both to an increase of TrKB-FL or a decrease of TrKB-T, or both.

Discussion

Appropriate levels of neurotrophins –and their specific receptors– are required for adequate neuronal response and survival. The results of this study reveal that cognitive performance improves when BDNF full-length receptor levels exceed those of truncated receptors. This finding supports our hypothesis that the role of neurotrophin receptors is more relevant than that of neurotrophins themselves. Neurotrophin receptors help neurotrophins modulate brain plasticity, which is related to cognitive performance. The increase of this index (FL/T ratio) in the first six months of treatment after a first psychotic episode is also associated with better functioning. When the FL/T ratio increases over time global functioning improves. Yet, to improve the cognitive performance of the patient, it is necessary that the expression of the functional receptor is greater than the expression of the truncated receptor. This may be due to the fact that cognitive processes generate free radicals, which requires a more effective repair system.

Increased TrkB-FL levels in the long term with respect to the truncated form may be an adaptive response to cellular damage at disease onset. In a previous study, we observed that BDNF receptor expression at disease onset is related to patient’s response to treatment [6]. The reason why BDNF is not a reliable biomarker for cognition may be that it dilutes in plasma. Also, there is evidence that the role of BDNF as a marker of response to cognitive remediation in patients with schizophrenia depends on polymorphisms of the BDNF gene [19].

Inconsistent data have been published on peripheral BDNF levels in patients with schizophrenia [7,20,21] or a FEP[22,23].

Our results can be explained by several mechanisms. The truncated form of the TrKB (TrKB-T) receptor sequesters BDNF, competes with TrKB-FL, and uses a different signaling pathway [11]. In an in vitro study, Dorsey et al (2012) observed that synaptic function improves in the absence of TrKB-T [24]. This means that increased TrKB-T receptor levels impair synaptic function and inhibit the neurotrophic function of BDNF [25]. Indeed, BDNF overexpression prevents cognitive deterioration in adolescent cannabis users [26]. Moreover, BDNF-mediated cellular survival has been reported to decrease when TrKB-T is overexpressed in an animal model of Down’s syndrome [27].

Our study revealed that a higher educational level is related to better cognitive performance after a FEP [28]. Other studies in elderly population have documented that a high education level has as protective effect against cognitive impairment [29]. Additionally, higher education has been proven to be a predictor of global functioning and quality of life in patients with schizophrenia [30].

Negative results have been obtained in intervention studies where BDNF was administered to animals. These results may be due to difficulties in the administration of BDNF to animals. Also, its pharmacokinetics is unknown [31], as neurotrophins are large proteins with a poor penetration into tissue that cannot enter the blood-brain barrier. TrKB receptor activation is known to have neuroprotective effects [32]. Furthermore, there is evidence that TrKB receptors can be activated using low-molecular-weight molecules such as adenoside/adenosine agonists in neuronal populations with TrkB and adenosine receptor expression [33]. Glucocorticoids have also been shown to promote TrKB activation –especially in neurogenic CNS regions–, with no increase in BDNF expression in vitro and in vivo in animals [34]. Studies are warranted in the future to determine whether the TrkB receptor that activates in the absence of BDNF is TrKB-FL or TrKB-T.

Dorsey et al reversed neuronal death by restoring TrKB-FL expression in experimental animals [35]. Thus, TrKB-FL could be the mediating factor that explains that overall and cognitive functioning improves when TrKB-FL and TrKB-T ratio is adequate. Levels of BDNF receptor isoforms in areas related to cognition – such as the hippocampus– have been associated with cognitive decline in aged rats [36]. According to Dorsey, TrKB-T overexpression with respect to TrKB-FL is a disruptor of the BDNF signaling pathway.

Under normal conditions, TrKB-T expresses in the presence of exogenous BDNF to metabolize FL-BDNF complexes and regulate TrKB-FL activity [37]. In patients with schizophrenia, the BDNF signaling pathway may be altered from the first psychotic episode. According to Dorsey, TrKB-T overexpression with respect to TrKB-FL is a disruptor of the BDNF signaling pathway. Further studies based on the synthesis of these receptors (mRNA, PCR) are needed to determine changes in FL/T receptor expression.

A limitation of the study concerns evidence that BDNF serum levels and TrK-B receptors are related to traumatic experiences [38,39] and this factor has not been taken into account in this research. Furthermore, we have focused on one of the receptors of BDNF. The other receptor, p75, has not been measured in this study. Another limitation is that although the truncated form is considered nonfunctional in CNS, it inhibits p75 mediated neuronal death induced by BDNF.

The strengths of the study include that a highly homogeneous sample in terms of age, duration of illness, and pharmacological treatment. Strength is long-term follow-up.

In conclusion, levels of BDNF receptors during first episode psychosis and at two years are related to cognitive and overall functioning. Further research is warranted to explore if cognitive and functional deterioration in patients with psychosis can be prevented by an intervention on the BDNF signaling pathway.

Conflict of Interests

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competiveness and the Carlos III Health Institute through CIBERSAM, and the Foundation Santander-UCM (GR 58/08), and partly supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competiveness directly, and by the European Regional Development Fund, FEDER (PI10/01746, PI11/01977, PI11/02708, PI12/02077, PI14/01900, PI15/00299, PI15/00789, PI16/01164), local grants from the Department of Education, Linguistic Policy and Culture of the Basque Country Government (2010111170, 2010112009), the Basque Foundation for Health Innovation and Research (BIOEF), ) and the University of the Basque Country (GIC10/80, GIC12/84 (IT679-13))

We want to particularly acknowledge the patients enrolled in this study for their participation and the Basque Biobank for its collaboration

References